Humanity’s closest living relatives are common chimpanzees and bonobos. These primates share a common ancestor
with humans who lived between six and eight million years ago. It is
for this reason that chimpanzees and bonobos are viewed as the best
available surrogate for this common ancestor. Barbara King argues that
while non-human primates are not religious, they do exhibit some traits
that would have been necessary for the evolution of religion. These
traits include high intelligence, a capacity for symbolic communication, a sense of social norms, realization of "self" and a concept of continuity.[1][2] There is inconclusive evidence that Homo neanderthalensis may have buried
their dead which is evidence of the use of ritual. The use of burial
rituals is thought to be evidence of religious activity, and there is no
other evidence that religion existed in human culture before humans
reached behavioral modernity.[3]
Elephants actually demonstrate rituals around their deceased, which
includes long periods of silence and mourning at the point of death and a
process of returning to grave sites and caressing the remains.[4][5] Some evidence suggests that many species grieve death and loss.[6]
Setting the stage for human religion
Increased brain size
In
this set of theories, the religious mind is one consequence of a brain
that is large enough to formulate religious and philosophical ideas.[7] During human evolution, the hominid brain tripled in size, peaking 500,000 years ago. Much of the brain's expansion took place in the neocortex.
This part of the brain is involved in processing higher order cognitive
functions that are connected with human religiosity. The neocortex is
associated with self-consciousness, language and emotion[citation needed]. According to Dunbar's theory, the relative neocortex size of any species correlates with the level of social complexity of the particular species.[8]
The neocortex size correlates with a number of social variables that
include social group size and complexity of mating behaviors.[9] In chimpanzees the neocortex occupies 50% of the brain, whereas in modern humans it occupies 80% of the brain. Robin Dunbar argues that the critical event in the evolution of the neocortex took place at the speciation of archaic homo sapiens
about 500,000 years ago. His study indicates that only after the
speciation event is the neocortex large enough to process complex social
phenomena such as language and religion. The study is based on a regression analysis of neocortex size plotted against a number of social behaviors of living and extinct hominids.[10] Stephen Jay Gould
suggests that religion may have grown out of evolutionary changes which
favored larger brains as a means of cementing group coherence among
savannah hunters, after that larger brain enabled reflection on the
inevitability of personal mortality.[11]
Tool use
Lewis Wolpert
argues that causal beliefs that emerged from tool use played a major
role in the evolution of belief. The manufacture of complex tools
requires creating a mental image of an object which does not exist
naturally before actually making the artifact. Furthermore, one must
understand how the tool would be used, that requires an understanding of
causality.[12] Accordingly, the level of sophistication of stone tools is a useful indicator of causal beliefs.[13]
Wolpert contends use of tools composed of more than one component, such
as hand axes, represents an ability to understand cause and effect.
However, recent studies of other primates indicate that causality may
not be a uniquely human trait. For example, chimpanzees have been known
to escape from pens closed with multiple latches, which was previously
thought could only have been figured out by humans who understood
causality. Chimpanzees are also known to mourn the dead, and notice
things that have only aesthetic value, like sunsets, both of which may
be considered to be components of religion or spirituality.[14]
The difference between the comprehension of causality by humans and
chimpanzees is one of degree. The degree of comprehension in an animal
depends upon the size of the prefrontal cortex: the greater the size of
the prefrontal cortex the deeper the comprehension.[15]
Religion requires a system of symbolic communication, such as language, to be transmitted from one individual to another. Philip Lieberman states "human religious thought and moral sense clearly rest on a cognitive-linguistic base".[16] From this premise science writer Nicholas Wade states:
"Like most behaviors that are found in societies throughout the
world, religion must have been present in the ancestral human population
before the dispersal from Africa 50,000 years ago. Although religious
rituals usually involve dance and music, they are also very verbal,
since the sacred truths have to be stated. If so, religion, at least in
its modern form, cannot pre-date the emergence of language. It has been
argued earlier that language attained its modern state shortly before
the exodus from Africa. If religion had to await the evolution of
modern, articulate language, then it too would have emerged shortly
before 50,000 years ago."[17]
Another view distinguishes individual religious belief from
collective religious belief. While the former does not require prior
development of language, the latter does. The individual human brain has
to explain a phenomenon in order to comprehend and relate to it. This
activity predates by far the emergence of language and may have caused
it. The theory is, belief in the supernatural emerges from hypotheses
arbitrarily assumed by individuals to explain natural phenomena that
cannot be explained otherwise. The resulting need to share individual
hypotheses with others leads eventually to collective religious belief. A
socially accepted hypothesis becomes dogmatic backed by social
sanction.
Frans de Waal
and Barbara King both view human morality as having grown out of
primate sociality. Though morality awareness may be a unique human
trait, many social animals, such as primates, dolphins and whales, have been known to exhibit pre-moral sentiments. According to Michael Shermer, the following characteristics are shared by humans and other social animals, particularly the great apes:
"attachment and bonding, cooperation and mutual aid, sympathy and
empathy, direct and indirect reciprocity, altruism and reciprocal
altruism, conflict resolution and peacemaking, deception and deception
detection, community concern and caring about what others think about
you, and awareness of and response to the social rules of the group".[18]
De Waal contends that all social animals have had to restrain or
alter their behavior for group living to be worthwhile. Pre-moral
sentiments evolved in primate societies as a method of restraining
individual selfishness and building more cooperative groups. For any
social species, the benefits of being part of an altruistic group should
outweigh the benefits of individualism. For example, lack of group
cohesion could make individuals more vulnerable to attack from
outsiders. Being part of a group may also improve the chances of finding
food. This is evident among animals that hunt in packs to take down large or dangerous prey.
All social animals have hierarchical societies in which each member
knows its own place. Social order is maintained by certain rules of
expected behavior and dominant group members enforce order through
punishment. However, higher order primates also have a sense of
reciprocity and fairness. Chimpanzees remember who did them favors and
who did them wrong. For example, chimpanzees are more likely to share food with individuals who have previously groomed them.[citation needed]
Chimpanzees live in fission-fusion groups that average 50 individuals. It is likely that early ancestors of humans lived in groups of similar size. Based on the size of extant hunter-gatherer societies, recent Paleolithic hominids
lived in bands of a few hundred individuals. As community size
increased over the course of human evolution, greater enforcement to
achieve group cohesion would have been required. Morality may have
evolved in these bands of 100 to 200 people as a means of social
control, conflict resolution and group solidarity. According to Dr. de
Waal, human morality has two extra levels of sophistication that are not
found in primate societies. Humans enforce their society’s moral codes
much more rigorously with rewards, punishments and reputation building.
Humans also apply a degree of judgment and reason not otherwise seen in
the animal kingdom.
Psychologist Matt J. Rossano argues that religion emerged after
morality and built upon morality by expanding the social scrutiny of
individual behavior to include supernatural
agents. By including ever-watchful ancestors, spirits and gods in the
social realm, humans discovered an effective strategy for restraining
selfishness and building more cooperative groups.[19] The adaptive value of religion would have enhanced group survival.[20][21]
Rossano is referring here to collective religious belief and the social
sanction that institutionalized morality. According to Rossano's
teaching, individual religious belief is thus initially epistemological,
not ethical, in nature.
There is general agreement among cognitive scientists that religion
is an outgrowth of brain architecture that evolved early in human
history. However, there is disagreement on the exact mechanisms that
drove the evolution of the religious mind. The two main schools of
thought hold that either religion evolved due to natural selection and has selective advantage, or that religion is an evolutionary byproduct of other mental adaptations.[22]Stephen Jay Gould, for example, believed that religion was an exaptation or a spandrel, in other words that religion evolved as byproduct of psychological mechanisms that evolved for other reasons.[23][24][25]
Such mechanisms may include the ability to infer the presence of
organisms that might do harm (agent detection), the ability to come up
with causal narratives for natural events (etiology), and the ability to recognize that other people have minds of their own with their own beliefs, desires and intentions (theory of mind).
These three adaptations (among others) allow human beings to imagine
purposeful agents behind many observations that could not readily be
explained otherwise, e.g. thunder, lightning, movement of planets,
complexity of life, etc.[26] The emergence of collective religious belief identified the agents as deities that standardized the explanation.[27]
Some scholars have suggested that religion is genetically "hardwired" into the human condition. One controversial proposal, the God gene hypothesis, states that some variants of a specific gene, the VMAT2 gene, predispose to spirituality.[28]
Another view is based on the concept of the triune brain: the reptilian brain, the limbic system, and the neocortex, proposed by Paul D. MacLean.
Collective religious belief draws upon the emotions of love, fear, and
gregariousness and is deeply embedded in the limbic system through
socio-biological conditioning and social sanction. Individual religious
belief utilizes reason based in the neocortex and often varies from
collective religion. The limbic system is much older in evolutionary
terms than the neocortex and is, therefore, stronger than it much in the
same way as the reptilian is stronger than both the limbic system and
the neocortex.
Yet another view is that the behavior of people who participate in a
religion makes them feel better and this improves their fitness, so that
there is a genetic selection in favor of people who are willing to
believe in religion. Specifically, rituals, beliefs, and the social
contact typical of religious groups may serve to calm the mind (for
example by reducing ambiguity and the uncertainty due to complexity) and
allow it to function better when under stress.[29] This would allow religion to be used as a powerful survival mechanism, particularly in facilitating the evolution of hierarchies of warriors, which if true, may be why many modern religions tend to promote fertility and kinship.
Still another view, proposed by F.H. Previc, is that human religion
was a product of an increase in dopaminergic functions in the human
brain and a general intellectual expansion beginning around 80 kya.[30][31][32] Dopamine promotes an emphasis on distant space and time, which is critical for the establishment of religious experience.[33]
While the earliest shamanic cave paintings date back around 40 kya, the
use of ochre for rock art predates this and there is clear evidence for
abstract thinking along the coast of South Africa by 80 kya.
The
earliest evidence of religious thought is based on the ritual treatment
of the dead. Most animals display only a casual interest in the dead of
their own species.[34]
Ritual burial thus represents a significant change in human behavior.
Ritual burials represent an awareness of life and death and a possible
belief in the afterlife. Philip Lieberman states "burials with grave goods clearly signify religious practices and concern for the dead that transcends daily life."[16]
The earliest evidence for treatment of the dead comes from Atapuerca in Spain. At this location the bones of 30 individuals believed to be Homo heidelbergensis have been found in a pit.[35]Neanderthals are also contenders for the first hominids
to intentionally bury the dead. They may have placed corpses into
shallow graves along with stone tools and animal bones. The presence of
these grave goods
may indicate an emotional connection with the deceased and possibly a
belief in the afterlife. Neanderthal burial sites include Shanidar in Iraq and Krapina in Croatia and Kebara Cave in Israel.[36][37][37][38]
The earliest known burial of modern humans is from a cave in Israel located at Qafzeh. Human remains have been dated to 100,000 years ago. Human skeletons were found stained with red ochre.
A variety of grave goods were found at the burial site. The mandible of
a wild boar was found placed in the arms of one of the skeletons.[39] Philip Lieberman states:
"Burial rituals incorporating grave goods may have been invented by
the anatomically modern hominids who emigrated from Africa to the Middle
East roughly 100,000 years ago".[39]
Matt Rossano suggests that the period between 80,000–60,000 years
before present, following the retreat of humans from the Levant to
Africa, was a crucial period in the evolution of religion.[40]
The use of symbolism
The use of symbolism in religion is a universal established phenomenon. Archeologist Steven Mithen
contends that it is common for religious practices to involve the
creation of images and symbols to represent supernatural beings and
ideas. Because supernatural beings violate the principles of the natural
world, there will always be difficulty in communicating and sharing
supernatural concepts with others. This problem can be overcome by
anchoring these supernatural beings in material form through
representational art. When translated into material form, supernatural
concepts become easier to communicate and understand.[41]
Due to the association of art and religion, evidence of symbolism in
the fossil record is indicative of a mind capable of religious thoughts.
Art and symbolism demonstrates a capacity for abstract thought and
imagination necessary to construct religious ideas. Wentzel van
Huyssteen states that the translation of the non-visible through
symbolism enabled early human ancestors to hold beliefs in abstract
terms.[42]
Some of the earliest evidence of symbolic behavior is associated with Middle Stone Age sites in Africa. From at least 100,000 years ago, there is evidence of the use of pigments such as red ochre.
Pigments are of little practical use to hunter gatherers, thus evidence
of their use is interpreted as symbolic or for ritual purposes. Among
extant hunter gatherer populations around the world, red ochre is still
used extensively for ritual purposes. It has been argued that it is
universal among human cultures for the color red to represent blood,
sex, life and death.[43]
The use of red ochre as a proxy for symbolism is often criticized as being too indirect. Some scientists, such as Richard Klein and Steven Mithen,
only recognize unambiguous forms of art as representative of abstract
ideas. Upper paleolithic cave art provides some of the most unambiguous
evidence of religious thought from the paleolithic. Cave paintings at Chauvet depict creatures that are half human and half animal.
Organized religion traces its roots to the neolithic revolution that began 11,000 years ago in the Near East
but may have occurred independently in several other locations around
the world. The invention of agriculture transformed many human societies
from a hunter-gatherer lifestyle to a sedentary lifestyle.
The consequences of the neolithic revolution included a population
explosion and an acceleration in the pace of technological development.
The transition from foraging bands to states and empires precipitated
more specialized and developed forms of religion that reflected the new
social and political environment. While bands and small tribes possess
supernatural beliefs, these beliefs do not serve to justify a central
authority, justify transfer of wealth or maintain peace between
unrelated individuals. Organized religion emerged as a means of
providing social and economic stability through the following ways:
Justifying the central authority, which in turn possessed the right
to collect taxes in return for providing social and security services.
Bands and tribes consist of small number of related individuals.
However, states and nations are composed of many thousands of unrelated
individuals. Jared Diamond
argues that organized religion served to provide a bond between
unrelated individuals who would otherwise be more prone to enmity. In
his book Guns, Germs, and Steel he argues that the leading cause of death among hunter-gatherer societies is murder.[44]
Religions that revolved around moralizing gods may have facilitated
the rise of large, cooperative groups of unrelated individuals.[45]
The states born out of the Neolithic revolution, such as those of Ancient Egypt and Mesopotamia, were theocracies with chiefs, kings and emperors playing dual roles of political and spiritual leaders.[18]
Anthropologists have found that virtually all state societies and
chiefdoms from around the world have been found to justify political
power through divine authority. This suggests that political authority
co-opts collective religious belief to bolster itself.[18]
Following the neolithic revolution, the pace of technological
development (cultural evolution) intensified due to the invention of
writing 5000 years ago. Symbols that became words later on made
effective communication of ideas possible. Printing invented only over a
thousand years ago increased the speed of communication exponentially
and became the main spring of cultural evolution. Writing is thought to
have been first invented in either Sumeria or Ancient Egypt and was
initially used for accounting. Soon after, writing was used to record
myth. The first religious texts mark the beginning of religious history. The Pyramid Texts from ancient Egypt are one of the oldest known religious texts in the world, dating to between 2400–2300 BCE.[46][47][48]
Writing played a major role in sustaining and spreading organized
religion. In pre-literate societies, religious ideas were based on an oral tradition,
the contents of which were articulated by shamans and remained limited
to the collective memories of the society's inhabitants. With the advent
of writing, information that was not easy to remember could easily be
stored in sacred texts that were maintained by a select group (clergy).
Humans could store and process large amounts of information with writing
that otherwise would have been forgotten. Writing therefore enabled
religions to develop coherent and comprehensive doctrinal systems that
remained independent of time and place.[49]
Writing also brought a measure of objectivity to human knowledge.
Formulation of thoughts in words and the requirement for validation made
mutual exchange of ideas and the sifting of generally acceptable from
not acceptable ideas possible. The generally acceptable ideas became
objective knowledge reflecting the continuously evolving framework of
human awareness of reality that Karl Popper calls 'verisimilitude' – a stage on the human journey to truth.[50]
Lewis Wolpert (2006). Six impossible things before breakfast, The evolutionary origins of belief. New York: Norton. ISBN0-393-06449-2. with
regard to hafted tools, One would have to understand that the two
pieces serve different purposes, and imagine how the tool could be used,